国際誌論文データベース

日本の社会心理学者たちは,活発な研究活動を展開・公表しており,その成果は日本語による論文であれば例えば日本社会心理学会の機関誌である「社会心理学研究」等の学会誌に掲載され,また学術書として公刊されています.一方,当然のことながら学問に国境はなく,特に近年では国際的な論文誌や書籍にその成果が掲載されることも増えてきました.しかし,こうした国際的成果をくまなく知ることは,あまりにそのフィールドが広いためにあまり容易ではありませんでした.

そこで,このページでは,日本の社会心理学者による国際的な研究活動の成果を広く共有・広報するために,日本社会心理学会会員による国際査読誌や書籍に掲載された学術論文(2013年以降に公刊されたもの)を,会員の皆様からの自薦・他薦の情報提供にもとづいて,あるいは,広報委員が不定期にPsycINFO, GoogleScholarなどを使って渉猟して,掲載しています.書誌情報は,メールニュース等の媒体でもご案内します.

  • 情報を掲載している論文を登録順(最新のものが上)でリストしています.
  • ページ右上「このサイトを検索」から,キーワード検索が可能です.

会員の皆様

掲載決定・刊行予定となった論文や書籍(分担執筆等も含む)をこちらのフォームから是非お知らせください.
既に登録した情報への追加・修正依頼は広報委員会( jssp_pr[at]googlegroups.com )([at]を@に変えてください。)に直接ご連絡ください.

現在の掲載論文数は,539件です.


Nakashima et al. (2015)

Nakashima, S. F.(中嶋智史), Ukezono, M(請園正敏), Nishida, H., Sudo, R.(須藤竜之介) , Takano, Y. (2015).
Receiving of emotional signal of pain from conspecifics in laboratory rats.
実験室ラットにおける同種個体の痛みの情動的シグナル知覚
Royal Society Open Science.
doi: 10.1098/rsos.140381
Though recent studies have shown that rodents express emotions with their face, whether emotional expression in rodents has a communicative function between conspecifics is still unclear. Here, we demonstrate the ability of visual recognition of emotional expressions in laboratory rats. We found that Long-Evans rats avoid images of pain expressions of conspecifics but not those of neutral expressions. The results indicate that rats use visual emotional signals from conspecifics to adjust their behaviour in an environment to avoid a potentially dangerous place. Therefore, emotional expression in rodents, rather than just a mere ‘expression’ of emotional states, might have a communicative function.
本研究では,ラットが表出された他個体の表情を視覚的に識別することができるかを検討した。実験では,刺激として,同一個体の中性表情写真と,フットショックを与えられた時の痛み表情写真を用い,実験動物がどちらの写真により長く接近するかを計測した。実験の結果,痛み表情に比べて,中性表情により長く接近していた。ラットが他個体の表情を視覚的に区別し,自己の将来の危機回避に利用している可能性が示唆された。

Kawakami & Yoshida (2015)

Kawakami, N.(川上直秋), & Yoshida, F.(吉田富二雄) (2015).
Perceiving a story outside of conscious awareness: When we infer narrative attributes from subliminal sequential stimuli. 
閾下呈示されたストーリーの知覚
Consciousness and cognition, 33, 53-66.
Perceiving a story behind successive movements plays an important role in our lives. From a general perspective, such higher mental activity would seem to depend on conscious processes. Using a subliminal priming paradigm, we demonstrated that such story perception occurs without conscious awareness. In the experiments, participants were subliminally presented with sequential pictures that represented a story in which one geometrical figure was chased by the other figure, and in which one fictitious character defeated the other character in a tug-of-war. Although the participants could not report having seen the pictures, their automatic mental associations (i.e., associations that are activated unintentionally, difficult to control, and not necessarily endorsed at a conscious level) were shifted to line up with the story. The results suggest that story perception operates outside of conscious awareness. Implications for research on the unconscious were also briefly discussed.

Fujii et al. (2015)

Fujii, T.(藤井貴之), Takagishi, H.(高岸治人), Koizumi, M. & Okada, H. (2015).
The Effect of Direct and Indirect Monitoring on Generosity Among Preschoolers.
未就学児の寛容さに対する直接/間接モニタリングの効果
Scientific Reports.
doi: 10.1038/srep09025
The purpose of this study is to examine the effect of direct and indirect monitoring on generosity among five-year-old preschoolers and to reveal the primary motivation for their generosity. Forty-two preschoolers completed one-shot dictator games in Condition 1 while being monitored by the experimenter (the direct monitoring condition). In Condition 2, an image of staring eyes was displayed on the computer monitor (the indirect monitoring condition). In Condition 3, the computer monitor showed a picture of flowers (the non-monitoring condition). The results showed that while there was no difference between the mean levels of allocation in the indirect and non-monitoring conditions, the mean level of allocation in the direct monitoring condition was significantly higher than in the non-monitoring condition. These results showed that five-year-old preschoolers concerned with being monitored by, and receiving direct responses from, others tend to be more generous.

Kobayashi et al. (2015)

Kobayashi, T.(小林哲郎), Collet, C., Iyengar, S., & Hahn, K. S. (2015). 
Who Deserves Citizenship? An Experimental Study of Japanese Attitudes Toward Immigrant Workers. 
市民権付与にふさわしいのは誰?-移民労働者に対する日本人の態度に関する実験的研究
Social Science Japan Journal, 18(1), 3-22.
doi: 10.1093/ssjj/jyu035
 
Citizenship eligibility has emerged as a salient issue in Japan as population diversity has increased and the political system has aligned around debates over regional security and tabunka kyōsei (‘multicultural co-existence’). A predominantly Western literature suggests that sentiments toward immigrants are driven by economic self-interest and/or cultural identity. Such approaches, we argue, privilege groups as units of analysis when it is the personal attributes of immigrants that have a bearing on critical outcomes, like naturalization. This is particularly the case in Japan, where officials maintain considerable discretionary powers in assessing the worthiness of candidates for naturalization. Drawing upon a nationally representative sample of Japanese adults, we use an experimental design to assess the average citizen’s willingness to grant citizenship to hypothetical applicants. The results indicate that individual applicants are viewed more favorably than their groups or nations of origin, indicating a person-positivity bias. Korean workers are, ceteris paribus, viewed more favorably than workers from China. We also find that socioeconomic status and willingness to assimilate contribute equally to the decision to award citizenship. Yet as there is general support for high-status applicants, affluent Japanese evaluate lower status applicants more negatively, undermining the expectation of labor market competition.

Kobayashi et al.(2015)

Kobayashi, T.(小林哲郎), Boase, J., Suzuki, T., & Suzuki, T.(鈴木貴久) (2015).
Emerging from the cocoon? Revisiting the tele-cocooning hypothesis in the smartphone era.
スマホ時代の"Tele-Cocooning"仮説再考
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 
doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12116
The tele-cocooning hypothesis posits that mobile communication increases interaction with communication rich ties, while simultaneously weakening interaction with communication weak ties. In this study, we demonstrate how smartphones can be used to mitigate tele-cocooning behavior by stimulating interaction with communication weak ties. Using a smartphone application to collect non-identifying mobile communication log data, we conducted a field experiment with 193 Japanese participants. The treatment consisted of onscreen reminders designed to stimulate interaction with communication weak ties. The results indicate that the treatment promoted the activation of communication weak ties and the acquisition of information through those ties, suggesting that smartphones can be utilized to promote access to social capital.

Thomson, & Ito (2014)

Thomson, R., & Ito, N. (2014). 
Facebook advertisements for survey participant recruitment: Considerations from a multi-country study. 
調査参加者募集のためのフェイスブック広告:20カ国調査による考察
International Journal of Electronic Commerce Studies, 5(2), 199-218.
doi: 10.7903/ijecs.1175
Facebook’s global reach suggests good potential for recruiting research participants and collecting objective behavioral data for cross-cultural research. Previous literature suggests the usefulness of Facebook advertisements to recruit participants in single-country studies. However, Facebook advert use in multi-country studies has not yet been reported. Nor are there any reports about soliciting Facebook user data (via Facebook applications) using Facebook advertisements. This paper contributes to this gap in Internet research, reporting on the effectiveness of Facebook advertisements to recruit participants, and for soliciting anonymized Facebook user data, in a 20-country study about privacy concerns on Facebook. Over seven days, 399 Facebook users from 18 countries responded to country-targeted advertisements in 13 languages. Response rates (ratio of advert clicks to valid responses) per country varied from 0% up to 14%. Overall, two-thirds of the country response rates were below 5%, and many country samples were gender-biased due to confounding societal factors. We conclude that for multi-national studies, Facebook advertisements may have potential for simple participant recruitment for surveys, but has limitations for soliciting Facebook user data. For user data collection, methods such as Amazon Mechanical Turk and snowball sampling may be more effective, but can be limited in their international reach.

Kawamoto et al. (2015)

Kawamoto, T.(川本大史), Ura, M.(浦光博), & Nittono, H. (2015). 
Intrapersonal and interpersonal processes of social exclusion. 
社会的排除の個人内/対人過程
Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 9:62. 
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00062
 
People have a fundamental need to belong with others. Social exclusion impairs this need and has various effects on cognition, affect, and the behavior of excluded individuals. We have previously reported that activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) could be a neurocognitive index of social exclusion (Kawamoto et al., 2012, Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4, 11). In this article, we provide an integrative framework for understanding occurrences during and after social exclusion, by reviewing neuroimaging, electrophysiological, and behavioral studies of dACC and rVLPFC, within the framework of intrapersonal and interpersonal processes of social exclusion. As a result, we have indicated directions for future studies to further clarify the phenomenon of social exclusion from the following perspectives: (1) constructional elements of social exclusion, (2) detection sensitivity and interpretation bias in social exclusion, (3) development of new methods to assess the reactivity to social exclusion and (4) sources of social exclusion.

Hashimoto & Yamagishi (2014)

Hashimoto, H.(橋本博文), & Yamagishi, T.(山岸俊男) (2014).
Preference-expectation reversal in the ratings of independent and interdependent individuals: A USA–Japan comparison.独立的/相互依存的個人の評定における選好-期待の逆転:日米比較
Asian Journal of Social Psychology, Article first published online: 29 DEC 2014
doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12094
We predicted and supported the prediction that a ‘preference-expectation reversal’ would occur among Japanese but not among Americans. American and Japanese participants evaluated ideal-typical independent and interdependent persons on the negative–positive dimension, and estimated how others would evaluate these persons. They also indicated their preference for acting like each of the target persons; that is, which of the two target persons they would prefer to act like. Both the American and Japanese participants wanted to act like the typical independent person rather than the typical interdependent person. However, the Japanese participants expected that others would evaluate the interdependent person more positively than the independent person. This preference-expectation reversal was not observed among the American participants. Further analysis demonstrated that the American participants’ personal evaluations of the two targets were consistent with their preferences. The Japanese participants’ personal evaluation represented a compromise between their preferences and the expected responses of others. These results suggest that the culturally shared belief in Japan that interdependent persons would receive more positive evaluations than independent persons created an incentive for them to behave interdependently despite their preferences.

Tanaka et al. (2015)

Tanaka, H.(田中大貴), Yagi, A.(八木彩乃), Komiya, A.(小宮あすか), Mifune, N.(三船恒裕), & Ohtsubo, Y.(大坪庸介) (2015). 
Shame-Prone People Are More Likely to Punish Themselves: A Test of the Reputation-Maintenance Explanation for Self-Punishment.
恥を感じやすい人は自分を罰しがち
Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9(1), 1-7.
doi: 10.1037/ebs0000016
Recent experimental studies have accumulated evidence about self-punishment. In accordance with the evolutionary perspective that shame has a reputation-maintenance function, we speculated that shame would promote self-punishment. Accordingly, we tested whether proneness to shame would predict self-punishment. In the first phase of the experiment, 98 undergraduates completed the Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA), a standard measure of proneness to shame and guilt. About 2 months later, 50 of the original participants took part in a self-punishment experiment, in which they all unintentionally made an unfair resource allocation, and then had the opportunity to inflict self-punishment by abandoning some of the money they had allocated to themselves. The amount of money the participants relinquished was significantly correlated with their shame-proneness. The intensity of posttransgression shame mediated the effect of shame-proneness on self-punishment. These results provide support for the evolutionary theorization of shame as a reputation-maintenance emotion.

Sawaumi et al. (2015)

Sawaumi, T. (澤海崇文), Yamaguchi, S. (山口勧), Park, J., & Robinson, A. R. (2015).
Japanese control strategies regulated by urgency and interpersonal harmony: Evidence based on extended conceptual framework.
日本人のコントロール方略選択への状況切迫性と対人的調和の影響
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(2), 252-268.
doi: 10.1177/0022022114563749
 
People use control strategies to improve their physical as well as interpersonal situations. Previous research has maintained that Japanese, compared to North Americans, are more oriented toward secondary control (changing oneself) than primary control (changing one’s circumstances) (e.g., Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984). On the other hand, Heckhausen and Schulz’s (1995) work suggests dominance of primary control over secondary control across cultures. The conflicting views regarding Japanese control orientations are reconciled by considering situational variation. Based on an extended framework of primary control (Yamaguchi, 2001), two empirical studies examined the alternative hypothesis that control orientation would be affected by perceived urgency and concern about harmony maintenance. Study 1 employed open-ended questions (N = 171) to validate the extended primary control taxonomy, and revealed that participants’ control orientations were influenced by their subjective urgency of control and perceived difficulty in maintaining interpersonal harmony. Study 2 (N = 246) replicated the latter results with Likert scale ratings. These results support the extended framework of primary control and identify two situational predictors of control orientation.