国際誌論文データベース

日本の社会心理学者たちは,活発な研究活動を展開・公表しており,その成果は日本語による論文であれば例えば日本社会心理学会の機関誌である「社会心理学研究」等の学会誌に掲載され,また学術書として公刊されています.一方,当然のことながら学問に国境はなく,特に近年では国際的な論文誌や書籍にその成果が掲載されることも増えてきました.しかし,こうした国際的成果をくまなく知ることは,あまりにそのフィールドが広いためにあまり容易ではありませんでした.

そこで,このページでは,日本の社会心理学者による国際的な研究活動の成果を広く共有・広報するために,日本社会心理学会会員による国際査読誌や書籍に掲載された学術論文(2013年以降に公刊されたもの)を,会員の皆様からの自薦・他薦の情報提供にもとづいて,あるいは,広報委員が不定期にPsycINFO, GoogleScholarなどを使って渉猟して,掲載しています.書誌情報は,メールニュース等の媒体でもご案内します.

  • 情報を掲載している論文を登録順(最新のものが上)でリストしています.
  • ページ右上「このサイトを検索」から,キーワード検索が可能です.

会員の皆様

掲載決定・刊行予定となった論文や書籍(分担執筆等も含む)をこちらのフォームから是非お知らせください.
既に登録した情報への追加・修正依頼は広報委員会( jssp_pr[at]googlegroups.com )([at]を@に変えてください。)に直接ご連絡ください.

現在の掲載論文数は,539件です.


Tabuchi et al.(2015)

Tabuchi, M.(田渕恵), Nakagawa, T., Miura, A.(三浦麻子), & Gondo, Y. (2015).
Generativity and Interaction between the Old and Young: The Role of Perceived Respect and Perceived Reject.
The Gerontologist. First published online: November 22, 2013 
doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt135
Purpose of the Study: We examined the relationships among development ofgenerativity, generative acts, and psychological well-being in old age and the possible mediating effects of perceived rejection and respect from younger generations. We hypothesized that generative action would not lead to well-being unless responding elders perceived respect from younger generations, and, further, that perceived respect would predict the development of generativity.
Design and Methods: In Study 1, we sampled 252 older persons to verify our hypothetical model in a cross-sectional survey. We assessed generative concern, generative action, perceived rejection from younger people, and psychological well-being. In Study 2, the same measures and a new scale to measure perceived respect from younger people were administered in a longitudinal survey. Four hundred older adults responded at two time points 12 months apart.
Results: Path analysis showed that the effect of generative action on ill-being was mediated by perceived rejection and respect from younger generations. Perceived respect from younger generations at baseline predicted an increase in generative concern 12 months later.
Implications: The results suggest that positive intergenerational interaction leads togenerativity development and better well-being in old age.
高齢者の世代性(Generativity)発達および若年者への利他的行動の継続に,世代間相互作用が影響することを,パネルデータを用いて示した。これまでの先行研究では,高齢者と若年者の世代間交流により,世代性が発達し,高齢者における利他的行動が誘発されるとされてきたが,本研究により,高齢者が若年者から敬意や感謝といったポジティブなフィードバックを受けなければ,世代性発達や利他的行動継続が停滞することが示された。

Oda et al. (2014)

Oda, R., Machii, W., Takagi, S., Kato, Y., Takeda, M.(武田美亜), Kiyonari, T.(清成透子), Fukukawa, Y.(福川康之), Hiraishi, K.(平石界) (2013).
Personality and altruism in daily life.
Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 206-209.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.017
Personality may be among the factors contributing to individual differences in altruism. Given that explanations of altruistic behavior differ according to the relationship between actors and recipients, the personality traits contributing to altruist behavior may differ according to the relationship between the parties involved. However, few studies on the effect of personality on altruism have examined the relationship between donor and recipient, and no study has addressed altruistic behavior in daily life. We employed the Self-Report Altruism Scale Distinguished by the Recipient, which was newly developed to evaluate altruism among Japanese undergraduates, to investigate the relationship between the Big-Five personality traits and the frequency of altruistic behaviors toward various recipients (family members, friends or acquaintances, and strangers) in daily life. With the exception of extraversion, which commonly contributed to altruistic behavior toward all three types of recipients, the particular traits that contributed to altruism differed according to recipient. Conscientiousness contributed to altruism only toward family members, agreeableness contributed to altruism only toward friends/acquaintances, and openness contributed to altruism only toward strangers.
日常生活における利他行動とビッグ5パーソナリティとの関連を検討した。利他行動は「家族」「友人・知人」「他人」に対するものを区別して質問紙により測定した(小田ら, 2013, 心理学研究)。外向性は、利他行動の3次元全てに寄与していた。家族への利他行動には、誠実性も寄与していた。友人・知人への利他行動には、協調性も寄与していた。他人への利他行動には、開放性も寄与していた。

Kawamoto et al. (2013)

Kawamoto, T.(川本大史), Nittono, H., & Ura, M.(浦光博) (2013). 
Cognitive, affective, and motivational changes during ostracism: An ERP, EMG, and EEG study using a computerized Cyberball task.
Neuroscience Journal, vol. 2013, Article ID 304674, 11 pages. 
doi: 10.1155/2013/304674
Individuals are known to be highly sensitive to signs of ostracism, such as being ignored or excluded, however, the cognitive, affective, and motivational processes underlying ostracism have remained unclear. We investigated temporal changes in these psychological states resulting from being ostracized by a computer. Using event-related brain potentials (ERP), the facial electromyogram (EMG), and electroencephalogram (EEG), we focused on the P3b amplitude, corrugator supercilii activity, and frontal EEG asymmetry, which reflect attention directed at stimuli, negative affect, and approach/withdrawal motivation, respectively. Results of the P3b and corrugator supercilii activity replicated findings of previous studies on being ostracized by humans. The mean amplitude of the P3b wave decreased, and facial EMG activity increased over time. In addition, frontal EEG asymmetry changed from relative left frontal activation, suggestive of approach motivation to relative right frontal activation, indicative of withdrawal motivation. These findings suggest that ostracism by a computer-generated opponent is an aversive experience that in time, changes the psychological status of ostracized people, similar to ostracism byhuman. Our findings also imply that frontal EEG asymmetry is a useful index for investigating ostracism. Results of this study suggest that ostracism has well developed neurobiological foundations.

Tanaka (2013)

Tanaka, Y. (田中豊)
Attitude gaps between conventional plant breeding crops and GM crops, and psychological models determining the acceptance of the two crops.
Journal of Risk Research, 16, 69-80.
doi10.1080/13669877.2012.726236
 
Thus far, people’s attitudes toward biotechnology and genetically modified (GM) crops have been investigated in the European Union (EU), United States (US), Japan, and other nations. However, direct comparisons between people’s attitudes toward conventional plant breeding crops and their attitudes toward GM crops will show the characteristics of people’s attitudes toward GM crops more clearly, and this will bring about valuable suggestions considering risk communication about GM crops. The first purpose of this study was to show attitude gaps between conventional plant breeding crops and GM crops. Referring to preceding studies related to the acceptance of biotechnology, a causal model was constructed to predict the acceptance of GM crops and plant breeding crops. Four factors—perceived risk, perceived benefit, trust, and sense of bioethics—were set up as important psychological factors determining the acceptance of GM crops and plant breeding crops in the structural equation models. The second purpose of this study was to verify the validity of these causal models. A social survey was administered in Tokyo, Japan, with a sample of 600 men and women aged 20 years or older who participated. The results showed that people have a more negative attitude toward GM crops than toward conventional plant breeding crops in every factor, that is, perceived risk, perceived benefit, trust, sense of bioethics, and acceptance. The plausibility of the causal models was tested by using structural equation modelling (SEM), and the results indicated that the four factors explain the acceptance very well in both models. Moreover, although perceived risk is the most important factor in the acceptance of GM crops, trust is the most important factor in the acceptance of conventional plant breeding crops.
 
これまで遺伝子組換え農作物に対する人々の態度が、ヨーロッパや米国、日本やその他の国々において調査されてきた。 品種改良農作物に対する人々の態度と遺伝子組換え農作物に対する態度とを直接比較することによって、遺伝子組換え農作物に対する人々の態度の特徴をよりはっきりと示すことができ、またこのことは遺伝子組換え農作物のリスクコミュニケーションについて考える上でも有意義な示唆を与えるであろう。本研究の第1の目的は、品種改良農作物に対する人々の態度と遺伝子組換え農作物に対する態度との違いを示すことであった。本研究の第2の目的は、遺伝子組換え農作物や品種改良農作物の受容を、リスク認知、ベネフィット認知、信頼、生命倫理観の4つの因子から予測する因果モデルを作成し、これらの因果モデルの妥当性を検証することであった。調査協力者として東京在住の20歳以上の男女600名をランダムサンプリングを用いて抽出し、社会調査を実施した。 その結果まず、リスク認知、ベネフィット認知、信頼、生命倫理観、受容の全ての因子について、人々は遺伝子組換え農作物に対して、品種改良農作物に対するよりもより否定的な態度を取っていることが明らかにされた。因果モデルの妥当性を構造方程式モデリング(SEM)を用いて検証したところ、上述の4つの因子は、遺伝子組換え農作物の受容および品種改良農作物の受容のどちらも良く説明することが示された。さらに、遺伝子組換え農作物の受容においてはリスク認知が最も重要であるが、品種改良農作物の受容においては信頼が最も重要であることなども示された。

Sato & Itakura (2013)

Sato, A. (佐藤徳) & Itakura, S. (2013).
Intersubjective action-effect binding: Eye contact modulates acquisition ofbidirectional association between our and others’ actions.
Cognition, 127(3), 383-390.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.010
In everyday social life, we predict others’ actions in response to our own actions. Subsequently, on the basis of these predictions, we control our actions to attain desired social outcomes and/or adjust our actions to accommodate the anticipated actions of the others. Representation of the bidirectional association between ourand others’ actions, that is, intersubjective action-effect binding, could make suchintersubjective action control easier and smoother. The present study investigated not only whether or not intersubjective action-effect binding was acquired but also whether or not eye contact modulated it. Experiment 1 showed that after a repeated experience during which participants’ finger movements triggered a target female individual’s mouth gesture, observing the target’s mouth gestures came to automatically trigger the participants’ finger movements. Experiments 2 and 3 revealed that this effect was not observed when the target’s gaze direction was averted (Experiment 2) or when the target’s eyes were closed (Experiment 3) throughout the acquisition phase. These results indicate that intersubjective action-effect binding occurs and that an ostensive signal, that is, eye contact modulates it.

Kimura et al. (2013)

Kimura, K., Murayama, A.(村山綾), Miura, A.(三浦麻子), & Katayama, J. (2013).
Effect of Decision Confidence on the Evaluation of Conflicting Decisions in a Social Context.
Neuroscience Letters, 556, 176-180.
Conflicts with others about decisions are reflected in the medial frontal negativity (MFN), which is a component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). The present study examined how decision confidence in a perceptual decision task modulated the ERP components elicited by decision conflicts with others. In a motion discrimination task, participants were randomly assigned to an easy or a difficult group that differed in coherence levels of the moving dots. Participants were paired with a confederate and presented the confederate’s decision about the motion direction of the moving dots, which was either congruent or incongruent with the participants’ decision. MFN was elicited by the incongruent confederate’s decision and its amplitude did not differ between groups. A larger P300 amplitude was elicited by incongruent decisions in the easy group, whereas the P300 amplitude did not differ between congruent and incongruent decisions in the difficult group. These results indicate the functional dissociation between MFN and P300 in the evaluation of social feedback. It is suggested that MFN might reflect automatic detection of conflicts about decisions,whereas the P300 might detect the deviation from internal expectations regarding other’s decisions.
近年,個人の意見と集団の意見との不一致が生じたときには,失敗行動の処理やエラー検出を反映する事象関連脳電位(ERP)である内側前頭陰性電位(Medial Frontal Negativity:MFN)および注意の配分を反映するP300が生起することが報告されている。本研究では,対人状況において知覚的意思決定を行う課題を用いることで,対人葛藤を生起するとともに実験的に判断の確信度を操作し,確信度が対人葛藤の処理に及ぼす影響を明らかにすることを目的とした。難易度高群もしくは低群にランダムに割り当てられた男子大学生26名が他者(サクラ)とペアを組み、ランダムドット刺激の運動方向を判断する知覚的意思決定課題を行った。両者の正答数の合算が実験後に与えられる金銭報酬になるため、できる限り正確に課題を遂行するように教示された。他者の判断は,参加者の反応に関わらず等頻度で一致あるいは不一致するよう操作した。他者の判断が提示された時点を刺激オンセットとして算出したERPを対象に分析を行った結果、両群においてMFNが観察され,対人葛藤が失敗やエラーとして処理されることが示された。また,判断の確信度はMFNに影響しておらず,対人葛藤の処理は自動的であることが示唆された。最後に,難易度低群のP300においてのみ一致条件と不一致条件に条件差が観察されたことから,判断の確信度は,対人葛藤が自動的に検出された後の処理段階を調整することが明らかになった。

Senzaki et al. (2014)

Senzaki, S., Masuda, T.(増田貴彦), & Ishii, K.(石井敬子) (in press).
When is perception top-down and when is it not? Culture, narrative, and attention.
Cognitive ScienceArticle first published online: 19 MAR 2014
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12118
Previous findings in cultural psychology indicated that East Asians are more likely than North Americans to be attentive to contextual information (e.g., Nisbett &Masuda, 2003). However, to what extent and in which conditions culture influences patterns of attention has not been fully examined. As a result, universal patterns of attention may be obscured, and culturally unique patterns may be wrongly assumed to be constant across situations. By carrying out two cross-cultural studies, we demonstrated that (a) both European Canadians and Japanese attended to moving objects similarly when the task was to simply observe the visual information; however, (b) there were cultural variations in patterns of attention when participants actively engaged in the task by constructing narratives of their observation (narrative construction). These findings suggest that cultural effects are most pronounced in narrative construction conditions, where the need to act in accordance with a culturally shared meaning system is elicited.
過去の文化心理学研究では、北米文化圏の人々に比べ、東アジア文化圏の人々は、事象の背景要因にまで注意を向ける傾向があることが報告されている(たとえば、Masuda & Nisbett, 2001; Nisbett & Masuda, 2003)。しかし、一方で、人間が文化普遍的にもつ注意のパターンの存在(たとえば目立ったものに目がいきがちである、など)があることも確実である。本研究では、日本人とヨーロッパ系カナダ人を対象に、魚のアニメーションのビデオを用いた視覚課題に従事してもらい、どのような場合に文化普遍的な傾向が見られ、どのような場合に文化特有の注意のパターンが見られるのかを検討した。その結果、アニメーションを単に見るだけの課題では、日本人もカナダ人もアニメーション内の目立った情報(きれいな魚、など)に大方の注意の向けがちであり、文化差はなかったが、アニメーションの内容を報告する課題になると、日本人はカナダ人に比べ、背景情報により注意を向けるというパターンがみられた。文化特有の注意のパターンを作り上げる、それぞれの文化構成員間でのナラティブおよび言語コミュニケーションの役割が論じられている。

Ishii (2013)

Ishii, K.(石井敬子) (2013).
Culture and the mode of thought: A review.
Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 123-132.
doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12011
 
This review highlights studies examining cultural differences in the mode of thought. The theoretical framework of studies from a cross-cultural perspective suggests that views of self, social norms, communication practices, and modes of thought are interconnected. The independent view of self, dominant in Western cultures, promotes low-contextual communication and corresponds to an analytic and logical mode of thought. On the other hand, the interdependent view of self, dominant in East Asian cultures, promotes high-contextual communication and corresponds to a holistic and dialectical mode of thought. The expected cultural variations of the mode of thought have been supported by empirical findings regarding causal attribution, attention, reasoning, categorization, and comprehension of utterances. Moreover, it has become clear that cultural knowledge of ideas and beliefs, which are activated by linguistic symbols and icons, guides the corresponding psychological tendencies including analytic and holistic modes of thought. Furthermore, studies of cultural neuroscience have shown cultural differences in behavioural as well as neural responses. A growing body of research supports cultural differences in the mode of thought; however, there is still much room for this research area to progress. Future directions suggesting further investigations in relation to socialization, cultural products, and gene–culture interaction are discussed.

Quintelier et al. (2013)

Quintelier, K. J. P., Ishii, K.(石井敬子), Weeden, J., Kurzban, R., & Braeckman, J. (2013).
Individual differences in reproductive strategy are related to views about recreational drug use in Belgium, the Netherlands and Japan.
Human Nature, 24, 196-217.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-013-9165-0
Individual differences in moral views are often explained as the downstream effect of ideological commitments, such as political orientation and religiosity. Recent studies in the U.S. suggest that moral views about recreational drug use are also influenced by attitudes toward sex and that this relationship cannot be explained by ideological commitments. In this study, we investigate student samples from Belgium, The Netherlands, and Japan. We find that, in all samples, sexual attitudes are strongly related to views about recreational drug use, even after controlling for various ideological variables. We discuss our results in light of reproductive strategies as determinants of moral views.

Endo et al. (2013)

Endo, G., Tachikawa, H., Fukuoka, Y. (福岡欣治), Aiba, M.(相羽美幸), Nemoto, K., Shiratori, Y., Matsui, Y. (松井豊), Doi, D., & Asada, T. (2013)
How perceived social support relates to suicaidal ideation: A Japanese social resident survey.
International Journal of Social Psychiatry, first published on June 5, 2013.
doi: 10.1177/0020764013486777
BACKGROUND: The loss of social support is one of the major risk factors for suicide. However, there are few empirical studies that have examined how a person’s suicide ideation relates to their social support.
AIMS: To examine the relationship between social support and suicidal ideation.
METHODS: Self-report questionnaires were sent to 2,200 randomly selected adults in Japan. The questionnaire inquired the participants about the severity of suicidal ideation, the details of current perceived social support and their degree of satisfaction with this social support. Social support and related indicators were compared among three groups of participants that varied in severity of suicidal ideation.
RESULTS: People in the group that had suicide ideation during their lives reported receiving significantly less support from their family and had greater feelings of dissatisfaction with that support than those in the other groups. Furthermore, people who had suicide ideation during the month immediately preceding the survey reported providing less support to their family, relatives or friends, as well as receiving less support from family than other groups, and having stronger feelings of dissatisfaction with social support.
CONCLUSION: Our study identified a strong relationship between the severity of suicidal ideation and perceived social support.