日本の社会心理学者たちは,活発な研究活動を展開・公表しており,その成果は日本語による論文であれば例えば日本社会心理学会の機関誌である「社会心理学研究」等の学会誌に掲載され,また学術書として公刊されています.一方,当然のことながら学問に国境はなく,特に近年では国際的な論文誌や書籍にその成果が掲載されることも増えてきました.しかし,こうした国際的成果をくまなく知ることは,あまりにそのフィールドが広いためにあまり容易ではありませんでした.
そこで,このページでは,日本の社会心理学者による国際的な研究活動の成果を広く共有・広報するために,日本社会心理学会会員による国際査読誌や書籍に掲載された学術論文(2013年以降に公刊されたもの)を,会員の皆様からの自薦・他薦の情報提供にもとづいて,あるいは,広報委員が不定期にPsycINFO, GoogleScholarなどを使って渉猟して,掲載しています.書誌情報は,メールニュース等の媒体でもご案内します.
- 情報を掲載している論文を登録順(最新のものが上)でリストしています.
- ページ右上「このサイトを検索」から,キーワード検索が可能です.
会員の皆様
掲載決定・刊行予定となった論文や書籍(分担執筆等も含む)をこちらのフォームから是非お知らせください.
既に登録した情報への追加・修正依頼は広報委員会( jssp_pr[at]googlegroups.com )([at]を@に変えてください。)に直接ご連絡ください.
現在の掲載論文数は,551件です.
Shimizu, Y. (2023).
Shimizu, Y.(清水佑輔) (2023). The effects of seeing an older person who violated or observed prescriptive stereotypes on ageism. 高齢者への規範的ステレオタイプに反する/遵守する高齢者について知ることがエイジズムに及ぼす影響 International Journal of Gerontology, 17(2), 144-145. https://doi.org/10.6890/IJGE.202304_17(2).0013
People generally exhibit ageism toward older adults that adversely affects the older adults’ health status. The prescriptive stereotypes held by younger individuals, such as succession (i.e., older adults should not accumulate wealth and pass it on to the younger generation), consumption (i.e., older adults should not receive more social security than they need), and identity (i.e., older adults should behave in an age-appropriate manner without trying to look young), account for the ageism. Moreover, if younger people see an older person violating such stereotypes, their ageism toward them will increase. We conducted an online experiment with young Japanese participants (N = 931). Each participant read one of the six vignettes about an older person who violated/observed the succession/consumption/identity aspect of prescriptive stereotypes. Results reveal that except for a single case involving the succession stereotype, other participants who read the vignettes of an older person violating prescriptive stereotypes did not exhibit more ageism than those who read the vignettes of an older person who observed the stereotypes. However, this result might be due to the inadequate sample size, and further research is required.
Park, J., Joshanloo, M., & Utsugi, A. (2023).
Park, J. (パクジュナ), Joshanloo, M., & Utsugi, A. (2023). Acculturation attitudes and adaptation among South Korean residents in Japan: The mediating role of coping strategies. 日本の韓国人ニューカマーにおける文化受容態度と適応: 対処戦略の媒介的役割 International Journal of Intercultural Relations. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2023.101799
Acculturation processes and intergroup experiences of minority groups have been little studied in East Asian societies, including Japan. The number of migrants in Japanese society is steadily increasing, suggesting that the country is a new immigration destination in the 21st century. Therefore, further research on the acculturation processes of immigrants in Japan is warranted. This study examined the relationships among acculturation attitudes, coping strategies, and psychological adjustment in a sample of South Korean newcomers living in Japan. The results of this study support the integration hypothesis, which states that balanced acculturation attitudes that favor engagement in both the host and home cultures lead to higher levels of psychological and sociocultural adaptation. Assimilation attitudes did not contribute significantly to adaptation. Different coping strategies employed by individuals during interethnic discrimination mediated the links between individual acculturation attitudes and the two aspects of adaptation. By linking acculturation attitudes and relevant social behaviors, this study sheds light on the role of coping strategies as mediators of the relationships between acculturation attitudes and psychological and sociocultural adjustment in ethnic minority groups.
日本を含む東アジア社会では、マイノリティグループの文化変容の過程や集団間の経験はほとんど研究されていない。日本社会における移民の数は着実に増加しており、日本が21世紀における新たな移民先の一つであることを示唆している。したがって、日本における移民の文化変容の過程に関するさらなる研究が必要である。本研究では、日本に住む韓国人ニューカマーをサンプルに、文化変容態度、対処戦略、心理的適応の関係を検討した。本研究の結果は、統合仮説を支持するものであった。すなわち、ホスト文化と自文化の両方へ関与しようとするバランスのとれた文化変容態度は、より高いレベルの心理的・社会文化的適応をもたらすというものである。同化の態度は適応に大きく寄与しなかった。民族間差別の際に個人が採用する対処戦略の違いは、個人の文化変容態度と適応の2つの側面との間のリンクを媒介した。本研究は、文化変容態度と関連する社会的行動とを関連づけることで、民族的マイノリティにおける文化変容態度と心理的・社会文化的適応との関係の媒介としての対処戦略の役割に光を当てている。
Shimizu, Y., Takeuchi, M., Karasawa, K. (2023).
Shimizu, Y.(清水佑輔), Takeuchi, M.(竹内真純), Karasawa, K(唐沢かおり). (2023). Anti-old and anti-youth attitudes among older adults: Focusing on middle-aged and old age identity. 高齢者が抱く、高齢者および若者への否定的態度:中年アイデンティティや高齢者アイデンティティに着目して The Journal of Social Psychology, 163(2), 248-255. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2022.2061893
There is currently a lack of comprehensive scholarly information concerning the attitudes older people hold toward both older adults and the young. Using the social identity theory framework, this study investigated older identity issues including middle-aged identity and old age identity. We conducted an online survey of Japanese older participants (N = 301) and then implemented a Bayesian structural equation modeling to examine whether age and gender predicted middle-aged/old age identity in addition to whether middle-aged/old age identity predicted anti-old/anti-youth attitudes. Results showed the more strongly participants identified with being middle-aged the more positive their attitudes were toward old/young people, while they showed no significant relationship between old age identity and the attitudes. Regarding participant ages, the results found no significant relationship with middle-aged identity but a positive relationship with old age identity. These findings will contribute to psychological research aimed at reducing anti-old/anti-youth attitudes among older adults.
Kusumi, T., Miura, A., Ogura, K., & Nishikawa, K. (2023).
Kusumi, T.(楠見孝), Miura, A.(三浦麻子), Ogura, K.(小倉加奈代), & Nishikawa, K.(西川一二) (2023). Attitudes toward possible food radiation contamination following the Fukushima nuclear accident: A nine-year, ten-wave panel survey. 福島第一原発事故による食品放射線リスクへの態度:9年10波パネル調査による検討 Journal of Risk Research. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2023.2181856
After the Fukushima nuclear accident, we examined changes in risk perception regarding the radiation contamination of food and information-seeking behavior among residents of three regions progressively more distant from the disaster area, the Tokyo Metropolitan area to the Kansai area. We conducted a ten-wave panel survey and obtained data from 1,752 citizens six months to nine years after the accident. The results indicate that anxiety related to radioactive contamination, active information-seeking behavior, and avoidance of foods from affected areas decreased with time. Active information-seeking behavior and radiation-related knowledge were higher in the disaster-affected prefectures than in other areas. Conversely, avoidance of foods from affected areas was lower in affected prefectures than in the Kansai area. The credibility of government information increased from a considerably low level but did not reach the midpoint level. Multiple regression analysis, cross-lagged analysis, and structural equation modeling indicated that avoidance of foods from affected areas was promoted by anxiety related to radioactive contamination (experiential thinking/System 1) and inhibited by critical thinking attitudes (analytical thinking/System 2). Finally, we discussed the significance of risk literacy, which integrates risk-related knowledge, scientific literacy, media literacy, and critical thinking.
福島原発事故後、首都圏から関西圏へと被災地からの距離の異なる3地域の住民を対象に、食品の放射能汚染に関するリスク認知や情報探索行動の変化を調査した。10波パネル調査を実施し、事故から半年から9年後の市民1,752名からデータを得た。その結果、放射能汚染に関する不安、積極的な情報探索行動、被災地産食品の忌避が時間の経過とともに減少していたことが示された。積極的な情報探索行動や放射線に関する知識は、被災県で他の地域より高かった。逆に、被災地産食品の忌避は、関西圏よりも被災県で低下した。政府情報の信頼性は、かなり低い水準から上昇したが、中位水準には達しなかった。被災地の食品を避けることは、放射能汚染に関する不安(経験的思考/システム1)が促進し、批判的思考態度(分析的思考/システム2)が抑制している可能性が示された。リスクに関する知識、科学的リテラシー、メディアリテラシー、批判的思考を統合したリスクリテラシーの意義について考察した。
Horita Y. (2023).
Horita Y.(堀田結孝) (2023). Paranoid thinking and perceived competitive intention. パラノイア思考と競争的意図 PeerJ, 11:e15003 https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.15003
Paranoid thinking, that others are hostile, can be seen even in the general population. Paranoia is considered the expectation that others are competitors who aim to maximize the differences in payoffs rather than maximize their own payoffs. This study examined whether paranoia reflects the irrational belief that others have a competitive intention and is associated with avoiding perceived competition. We recruited 884 US residents via the Internet and conducted a modified Dictator Game, in which monetary allocation was carried out between the Dictator and the Recipient. The Dictator chooses either fair or competitive allocation while selecting the competitive allocation is irrelevant to increasing the Dictator’s payoffs. The Recipient decides whether to accept the Dictator’s decision or receive sure but low rewards. We found that Recipients with high-level paranoid thinking expected their opponent to select competitive allocation more than those with low levels, even when selecting it was costly for Dictators. Paranoid thinking was not associated with selecting sure rewards or competitive allocations. The results suggest that paranoia reflects the belief that others have a competitive intention but is not related to avoidance behavior against perceived threats and unilateral attacks.
Morii, D., Miura, A., & Komori, M. (2023).
Morii, D., Miura, A.(三浦麻子), & Komori, M. (2023). The impact of television on-air time on hand hygiene compliance behaviors during COVID-19 outbreak. 新型コロナウイルス感染禍における手指消毒遵守に及ぼすテレビ報道時間の効果 American Journal of Infection Control. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2023.03.001
Background
There is limited data to support the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on hand hygiene compliance among hospital visitors.
Methods
We monitored hand hygiene compliance among university hospital visitors in Osaka, Japan by direct observation, from December 2019 to March 2022. During this time, we measured the amount of coverage time dedicated to COVID-19 related news on the local public television channel and the number of confirmed cases and deaths.
Results
Over 148 days, 111,071 visitors hand hygiene compliance was monitored. The baseline compliance was 5.3% (213 of 4026) in December 2019. From late January 2020, compliance rose significantly to almost 70% in August 2020. It remained at a level of 70% to 75% until October 2021, after which, the compliance slowly declined to the mid-60% range. The number of newly confirmed cases and deaths were not related to the change in compliance, but the association between the on-air time of COVID-19-related news and compliance was statistically significant.
Conclusions
Hand hygiene compliance dramatically increased following the COVID-19 pandemic. The role of television in increasing hand hygiene compliance was significant.
・2019年12月~2022年3月末の大阪大学附属病院来院者の手指衛生実施状況を観察し、新型コロナウイルス感染症禍による急増(5%→70%)を確認。その後漸減するもコロナ禍以前より圧倒的に高い割合を維持。
・新型コロナの新規感染者数および死亡者数の推移は、手指衛生実施率の変遷と無関係。
・新型コロナ関連ニュースのテレビ放送時間が、手指衛生実施率の変遷と関連。
Ogihara, Y. (2023).
Ogihara, Y. (荻原祐二) (2023). Popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries: Further validation of unique names as an indicator of individualism. 個人主義的な国では人気のある名前が新生児に与えられる頻度が低い:個性的な名前が個人主義傾向を示す指標として妥当かに関する更なる検証 Current Research in Behavioral Sciences, 4, 100094. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100094
A prior study showed that popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries in European, Anglo-American (North American), and Oceanian cultures. However, the samples of the previous study were limited, and it did not examine other cultures, leaving it unclear whether the relationship is still found even when other cultures are included. It is important to confirm validity of indices cross-culturally. Thus, the present study included two unexamined cultures in the analyses: East Asian culture and Latin American culture. Following the previous study, I calculated the rates of popular baby names in Japan and Puerto Rico, and examined how the addition of these two regions affected the results. Analyses showed that the negative relationships between the rates of popular names and individualism scores were still salient. Therefore, this study further confirmed the validity of unique names as an indicator of individualism in more diverse cultural contexts. / ヨーロッパ・北アメリカ・オセアニア文化圏における個人主義的な国では、人気のある名前が新生児に与えられる頻度が低いことを、先行研究は示していた。しかし、先行研究のサンプルは限定されており、他の文化を含めてもその関係が見られるか不明であった。指標の妥当性を文化横断的に検証することが重要である。よって本研究では、これまで検討されていなかった東アジア文化(日本)とラテンアメリカ文化(プエルトリコ)を含めて分析を行った。先行研究に従い、日本とプエルトリコにおける人気のある新生児の名前の割合を算出し、その2つの地域を含めることで結果がどのように変わるのかを検討した。分析の結果、人気のある名前の割合と個人主義得点には、負の関連が依然として見られた。そのため本研究は、名前指標が個人主義指標として妥当であることを、より文化的に多様な文脈において再確認したと言える。
Buchanan, E. M. et al. (2023).
Buchanan, E. M., Lewis, S. C., Paris, B., Forscher, P. S., Pavlacic, J. M., Beshears, J. E., Drexler, S. M., Gourdon-Kanhukamwe, A., Mallik, P. R., Silan, M. A. A., Miller, J. K., IJzerman, H., Moshontz, H., Beaudry, J. L., Suchow, J. W., Chartier, C. R., Coles, N. A., Sharifian, M., Todsen, A. L., … Primbs, M. A. (2023). The Psychological Science Accelerator’s COVID-19 rapid-response dataset. The Psychological Science AcceleratorによるCOVID-19に対する行動・感情反応についての研究データセット Scientific Data, 10(1), 87. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-022-01811-7
In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, the Psychological Science Accelerator coordinated three large-scale psychological studies to examine the effects of loss-gain framing, cognitive reappraisals, and autonomy framing manipulations on behavioral intentions and affective measures. The data collected (April to October 2020) included specific measures for each experimental study, a general questionnaire examining health prevention behaviors and COVID-19 experience, geographical and cultural context characterization, and demographic information for each participant. Each participant started the study with the same general questions and then was randomized to complete either one longer experiment or two shorter experiments. Data were provided by 73,223 participants with varying completion rates. Participants completed the survey from 111 geopolitical regions in 44 unique languages/dialects. The anonymized dataset described here is provided in both raw and processed formats to facilitate re-use and further analyses. The dataset offers secondary analytic opportunities to explore coping, framing, and self-determination across a diverse, global sample obtained at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, which can be merged with other time-sampled or geographic data.
*調査票の日本語訳および日本データの収集を井隼経子(福岡工業大学)・石井辰典(日本女子大学)・国里愛彦(専修大学)・角南直幸(Erasmus University Rotterdam)・山田祐樹(九州大学)が担当しました(氏名は姓のアルファベット順)。この活動に際し、日本心理学会の「新型コロナウイルス感染拡大に関連した実践活動及び研究」の助成を受けました。
Suzuki, T., Yamamoto, H., Ogawa, Y.& Umetani, R. (2023).
Suzuki, T.(鈴木貴久), Yamamoto, H.(山本仁志), Ogawa, Y.(小川祐樹)& Umetani, R.(梅谷凌平) (2023). Effects of media on preventive behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic. コロナ禍における予防行動に対するメディアの効果 Humanities and Social Science Communications 10, 58. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-01554-9
The novel coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic required implementation of a variety of measures. In addition to pharmaceutical measures, such as vaccines, changing individuals’ nonpharmaceutical preventive behaviour is essential to prevent the spread of infection. In uncertain situations, such as a pandemic, media sources are important for guiding individuals’ decision-making behaviour. In this study, we examined the effects of media use on preventive behaviour during COVID-19. Earlier studies have shown that social networking service (SNS) browsing promotes preventive behaviour. However, those studies only assessed a single point during the early stages of the pandemic; therefore, the effects on ongoing preventive behaviour are unclear. Thus, a two-wave panel survey was conducted in 2020 and 2021 for an exploratory analysis of changes in the effects of media on individuals’ preventive behaviour over time. The results show that the effect of SNS browsing on preventing going out was confirmed only during the early stage of the pandemic and was not observed 1 year later. It is also shown that those who shifted from self-restraint to going out within 1 year were not affected by the type of media use, but by cognitive factors. As the situation changes during a pandemic, analyses that consider time-series changes are essential for gaining insights about the effects of media on the promotion and maintenance of continuous prevention behaviours.
コロナ禍における外出自粛に関するパネル調査の結果から、メディア接触やメディアに対する態度が外出自粛態度の変化に与える効果について検討を行った。2020年4月と2021年4月の2時点でパネル調査を行い、外出自粛とメディア接触に関する項目について測定した。各時点で外出程度が異なる3段階のクラスタを抽出し、クラスタごとの特徴を探索的に分析した。その結果から、SNSの閲覧が自粛に対して一貫して効果を持つことや、2020年と2021年では外出する人の特徴が異なっていることも示された。
Inoue, Y., Mifune, N., & Saijo, T. (2023).
Inoue, Y.(井上裕香子), Mifune, N.(三船恒裕), & Saijo, T. (2023). Positive reputation for altruism toward future generations regardless of the cost for current others. 現世代他者へのコストを伴う場合でも、将来世代への利他行動は良い評判を得る Frontiers in Psychology, 13: 895619. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.895619
Recently, altruism toward future generations (future altruism) has become a hot research topic. Although future altruism has been observed in several previous experiments, it is not yet clear when and why people are more likely to engage in future altruism. Drawing upon the empirical literature of reputation and cooperation, we predicted that future altruism brings reputational disadvantages. Accordingly, we investigated whether future altruism was evaluated positively or negatively by others in the current generation in two vignette studies (total N = 1,237). Contrary to our initial prediction, we found that future altruism was positively evaluated even when it decreased the payoff of the members of the current generation. The difference in the evaluation of future altruism, as opposed to unsustainable current-generation focused behavior, was most pronounced when people do not know how a future altruist allocates rewards among individuals in the current generation. However, the positive evaluation of future altruism did not stem from the expectation that future altruists would also be altruistic toward the current generation. These results indicated that reputational benefits (i.e., positive reputation from others in the current generation) promote future altruism.
将来世代への利他行動は、しばしば行為者だけでなく現世代全体にコストをもたらす。そのような場合に行為者が、「現世代の利益を減らした」として現世代他者から悪い評判を得るのか、それとも「(将来世代の)第三者に利他行動をした」として良い評判を得るのかは不明であった。
そこで本研究では、ISDG(Kamijo et al., 2017) という将来世代への利他行動を測定する経済ゲームを用い、意思決定者を評価させる場面想定法課題をオンラインで実施した。具体的には、意思決定者が将来世代に利他的な選択をすると、評価者である参加者を含めた「現世代全体」の利益が減る状況で、将来世代に利他的な選択をした意思決定者と現世代の利益を最大化する選択をした意思決定者それぞれを参加者に評価させた。その結果、将来世代に利他的な意思決定者の方がより高く評価された。ただし、意思決定者が「現世代全体」の利益をその後どう世代内で分配したかという情報を追加すると、将来世代への意思決定が評価に及ぼす効果が小さくなった。